What spurs one consumer to try a new product or service while another will stick with something familiar for years, even if it may not be the best on the market? Exactly how strong a pull does the status quo exert on consumers and why?
Alexander Chernev (Professor of Marketing at the Kellogg School of Management) has spent his career studying these and other aspects of consumer behavior. He found that a consumer’s personal goals, particularly their concern for safety and security, strongly influence how likely they are to switch to something new or stay with what they know. Chernev also discovered an intriguing new wrinkle in consumer behavior—people’s decisions to remain with a known entity do not stem from fear of loss as much as previous research suggested.
Chernev’s research involved asking people whether or not they would choose to invest in alternative funds, some with potentially higher yields than ones they already owned. Prevention-focused people—those concerned above all with safety and security—were more likely to stick with the status quo even when they were told that another option would most likely out-perform it. In contrast, promotion-focused people—those who focus on growth and development—were much more willing to venture into the unknown.
Underlying Motivations and the Status Quo
While previous research has hinted at this, Chernev&rsqrsquo;s study went a step further. He used evidence from three separate experiments to examine not only how consumers’ goals affect their preference for the status quo, but also their underlying motivation.
In one experiment, seventy-four undergraduate students were randomly induced into adopting either a prevention focus or a promotion focus. This was accomplished by having them answer a set of questions about their duties and obligations or, alternately, about their hopes and aspirations.
Some participants in the prevention-focused group were given the hypothetical choice of sticking with an investment fund they already owned that earned 7.1 percent interest the prior year and was expected to earn 8.15 percent this year, or switching to a new one that was expected to earn 8.68 percent this year. Only 75.7 percent chose the higher-yielding option when the choice was phrased this way. When the choice was phrased so that the higher-yielding option was also the status quo option, 97.3 percent of prevention-focused respondents chose it.
This was one of two experiments that showed that the reluctance of prevention-focused people to try new things is not solely linked to their fear of loss. After all, if loss were the only thing they worried about, what would stop them from choosing the investment that was predicted to earn them the highest return?
Regret Aversion
While previous research sparked a theory that loss aversion is the main reason prevention-focused people shy away from choosing new options, Chernev’s research proposes another reason: regret aversion. Studies have shown that people tend to blame actions that produce bad results more than they blame inactions that produce bad results (Spranca, Minsk, and Baron 1991). In other words, people who switch to a new investment fund and end up losing money will experience more regret than if they took no action and lost money on their old fund.
In another experiment, respondents were not given the option to re-invest in their old funds. They could either invest in a fund that was expected to earn 8.15 percent, invest in one expected to earn 8.65 percent, or make no choice and be randomly assigned to one or the other. When the decision was framed as a gain (both of the new options were expected to earn more money than the respondent’s old fund), only 2 percent of the promotion-focused respondents opted to make no choice. Of the prevention-focused respondents, 10.6 percent selected the no-choice option. When the decision was framed as a loss (both of the new options were expected to earn less money than the respondent’s old fund), 10.2 percent of the promotion-focused and 21.3 percent of the prevention-focused respondents opted to make no choice.
This experiment lends even more support to the notion that consumers—particularly prevention-focused consumers—are motivated to stick to the status quo by something other than a fear of loss. The greatest number of respondents begged out of making any choice at all during the scenario in which they had the most to lose.
Chernev’s research has important implications for understanding how loss-aversion influences choice. Specifically, it shows that personal goals have a significant effect on consumers’ choices, and that this effect is more pronounced for prevention-focused individuals. It also leaves the door open for further studies to evaluate whether there is a point at which consumers’ goals completely override their sensitivity to loss.
Further reading
Spranca, Mark, Elisa Minsk, and Jonathan Baron. 1991. “Omission and commission in judgment and choice.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 27(1): 76–105.
Chernev, Alexander. 2004. “Goal orientation and consumer preference for the status quo.” Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3): 557–565.
-
Will AI Eventually Replace Doctors?Maybe not entirely. But the doctor–patient relationship is likely to change dramatically.
-
What Is the Purpose of a Corporation Today?Has anything changed in the three years since the Business Roundtable declared firms should prioritize more than shareholders?
-
What Happens to Worker Productivity after a Minimum Wage Increase?A pay raise boosts productivity for some—but the impact on the bottom line is more complicated.
-
3 Tips for Reinventing Your Career After a LayoffIt’s crucial to reassess what you want to be doing instead of jumping at the first opportunity.
-
Why We Can’t All Get Away with Wearing Designer ClothesIn certain professions, luxury goods can send the wrong signal.
-
Why You Should Skip the Easy Wins and Tackle the Hard Task FirstNew research shows that you and your organization lose out when you procrastinate on the difficult stuff.
-
Which Form of Government Is Best?Democracies may not outlast dictatorships, but they adapt better.
-
6 Takeaways on Inflation and the Economy Right NowAre we headed into a recession? Kellogg’s Sergio Rebelo breaks down the latest trends.
-
How Are Black–White Biracial People Perceived in Terms of Race?Understanding the answer—and why black and white Americans may percieve biracial people differently—is increasingly important in a multiracial society.
-
When Do Open Borders Make Economic Sense?A new study provides a window into the logic behind various immigration policies.
-
How Old Are Successful Tech Entrepreneurs?A definitive new study dispels the myth of the Silicon Valley wunderkind.
-
How Has Marketing Changed over the Past Half-Century?Phil Kotler’s groundbreaking textbook came out 55 years ago. Sixteen editions later, he and coauthor Alexander Chernev discuss how big data, social media, and purpose-driven branding are moving the field forward.
-
Why Do Some People Succeed after Failing, While Others Continue to Flounder?A new study dispels some of the mystery behind success after failure.
-
How to Get the Ear of Your CEO—And What to Say When You Have ItEvery interaction with the top boss is an audition for senior leadership.
-
Understanding the Pandemic’s Lasting Impact on Real EstateWork-from-home has stuck around. What does this mean for residential and commercial real-estate markets?
-
Immigrants to the U.S. Create More Jobs than They TakeA new study finds that immigrants are far more likely to found companies—both large and small—than native-born Americans.
-
Podcast: What to Expect When Joining a Family-Owned BusinessThere are cons—but a lot of pros, too. On this episode of The Insightful Leader, we’ll explore what it’s like to work at a family business when you’re not a family member.